Everipedia Logo
Everipedia is now IQ.wiki - Join the IQ Brainlist and our Discord for early access to editing on the new platform and to participate in the beta testing.
Wernicke's area

Wernicke's area

Wernicke's area (/ˈvɛərnɪkə/ or /ˈvɛərnɪki/; German: [ˈvɛʁnɪkə]), also called Wernicke's speech area, is one of the two parts of the cerebral cortex that are linked to speech (the other is Broca's area). It is involved in the comprehension of written and spoken language (in contrast to Broca's area that is involved in the production of language). It is traditionally thought to be in Brodmann area 39,40, which is located in the superior temporal lobe in the dominant cerebral hemisphere (which is the left hemisphere in about 95% of right handed individuals and 60% of left handed individuals).

Damage caused to Wernicke's area results in receptive, fluent aphasia. This means that the person with aphasia will be able to fluently connect words, but the phrases will lack meaning. This is unlike non-fluent aphasia, in which the person will use meaningful words, but in a non-fluent, telegraphic manner.[1]

Wernicke's area
Details
LocationTemporal lobe of the dominant cerebral hemisphere.
ArteryBranches from the middle cerebral artery
Identifiers
MeSHD065813 [17]
NeuroNames1233 [18]
NeuroLex IDnlx_144087 [19]
FMA242178 [20]
Anatomical terms of neuroanatomy

Structure

Wernicke's area is classically located in the posterior section of the superior temporal gyrus (STG) in the (most commonly) left cerebral hemisphere. This area encircles the auditory cortex on the lateral sulcus (the part of the brain where the temporal lobe and parietal lobe meet).[2] This area is neuroanatomically described as the posterior part of Brodmann area 22.

However, there is an absence of consistent definitions as to the location.[3][4] Some identify it with the unimodal auditory association in the superior temporal gyrus anterior to the primary auditory cortex (the anterior part of BA 22).[5] This is the site most consistently implicated in auditory word recognition by functional brain imaging experiments.[6][7] Others include also adjacent parts of the heteromodal cortex in BA 39 and BA40 in the parietal lobe.[8]

While previously thought to connect Wernicke's area and Broca's area, new research demonstrates that the arcuate fasciculus instead connects to posterior receptive areas with premotor/motor areas, and not to Broca's area.[9] Consistent with the word recognition site identified in brain imaging, the uncinate fasciculus connects anterior superior temporal regions with Broca's area.[10]

Function

Right homologous area

Research using Transcranial magnetic stimulation suggests that the area corresponding to the Wernicke’s area in the non-dominant cerebral hemisphere has a role in processing and resolution of subordinate meanings of ambiguous words—such as ‘‘river’’ when given the ambiguous word "bank." In contrast, the Wernicke's area in the dominant hemisphere processes dominant word meanings (‘‘teller’’ given ‘‘bank’’).[11]

Modern views

Neuroimaging suggests the functions earlier attributed to Wernicke's area occur more broadly in the temporal lobe and indeed happen also in Broca's area.

Support for a broad range of speech processing areas was furthered by a recent study caried out at the University of Rochester in which American Sign Language native speakers were subject to MRI while interpreting sentences that identified a relationship using either syntax (relationship is determined by the word order) or inflection (relationship is determined by physical motion of "moving hands through space or signing on one side of the body"). Distinct areas of the brain were activated with the frontal cortex (associated with ability to put information into sequences) being more active in the syntax condition and the temporal lobes (associated with dividing information into its constituent parts) being more active in the inflection condition. However, these areas are not mutually exclusive and show a large amount of overlap. These findings imply that while speech processing is a very complex process, the brain may be using fairly basic, preexisting computational methods.[13]

Clinical significance

Aphasia

Wernicke's area is named after Carl Wernicke, a German neurologist and psychiatrist who, in 1874, hypothesized a link between the left posterior section of the superior temporal gyrus and the reflexive mimicking of words and their syllables that associated the sensory and motor images of spoken words.[14] He did this on the basis of the location of brain injuries that caused aphasia. Receptive aphasia in which such abilities are preserved is also known as Wernicke's aphasia. In this condition there is a major impairment of language comprehension, while speech retains a natural-sounding rhythm and a relatively normal syntax. Language as a result is largely meaningless (a condition sometimes called fluent or jargon aphasia).

While neuroimaging and lesion evidence generally support the idea that malfunction of or damage to Wernicke's area is common in people with receptive aphasia, this is not always so. Some people may use the right hemisphere for language, and isolated damage of Wernicke's area cortex (sparing white matter and other areas) may not cause severe receptive aphasia.[3][15] Even when patients with Wernicke's area lesions have comprehension deficits, these are usually not restricted to language processing alone. For example, one study found that patients with posterior lesions also had trouble understanding nonverbal sounds like animal and machine noises.[16] In fact, for Wernicke's area, the impairments in nonverbal sounds were statistically stronger than for verbal sounds.

References

[1]
Citation Linkwww.asha.org"Aphasia: Signs & Symptoms". American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[2]
Citation Linkopenlibrary.orgKennison, Shelia (2013). Introduction to language development. Los Angeles: Sage.
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[3]
Citation Link//www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1070943Bogen JE, Bogen GM (1976). "Wernicke's region—Where is it?". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 280 (1): 834–43. Bibcode:1976NYASA.280..834B. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.657.3681. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1976.tb25546.x. PMID 1070943.
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[4]
Citation Link//www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28334963Nakai, Y; Jeong, JW; Brown, EC; Rothermel, R; Kojima, K; Kambara, T; Shah, A; Mittal, S; Sood, S; Asano, E (2017). "Three- and four-dimensional mapping of speech and language in patients with epilepsy". Brain. 140 (5): 1351–1370. doi:10.1093/brain/awx051. PMC 5405238. PMID 28334963.
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[5]
Citation Link//www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1486459Démonet JF, Chollet F, Ramsay S, Cardebat D, Nespoulous JL, Wise R, Rascol A, Frackowiak R (December 1992). "The anatomy of phonological and semantic processing in normal subjects". Brain. 115 (Pt 6): 1753–68. doi:10.1093/brain/115.6.1753. PMID 1486459.
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[6]
Citation Link//www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22308358DeWitt I, Rauschecker JP (2012). "Phoneme and word recognition in the auditory ventral stream". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 109 (8): E505–E514. Bibcode:2012PNAS..109E.505D. doi:10.1073/pnas.1113427109. PMC 3286918. PMID 22308358.
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[7]
Citation Link//www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24404576DeWitt I, Rauschecker JP (2013). "Wernicke's area revisited: parallel streams and word processing". Brain Lang. 127 (2): 181–91. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2013.09.014. PMC 4098851. PMID 24404576.
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[8]
Citation Link//www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9648540Mesulam MM (June 1998). "From sensation to cognition". Brain. 121 (Pt 6): 1013–52. doi:10.1093/brain/121.6.1013. PMID 9648540.
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[9]
Citation Link//www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19690094Bernal B, Ardila A (September 2009). "The role of the arcuate fasciculus in conduction aphasia". Brain. 132 (Pt 9): 2309–16. doi:10.1093/brain/awp206. PMID 19690094.
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[10]
Citation Link//www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19004769Saur D, Kreher BW, Schnell S, Kümmerer D, Kellmeyer P, Vry MS, Umarova R, Musso M, Glauche V, Abel S, Huber W, Rijntjes M, Hennig J, Weiller C (November 2008). "Ventral and dorsal pathways for language". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 105 (46): 18035–18040. Bibcode:2008PNAS..10518035S. doi:10.1073/pnas.0805234105. PMC 2584675. PMID 19004769.
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[11]
Citation Link//www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19251255Harpaz Y, Levkovitz Y, Lavidor M (October 2009). "Lexical ambiguity resolution in Wernicke's area and its right homologue". Cortex. 45 (9): 1097–103. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2009.01.002. PMID 19251255.
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[12]
Citation Link//www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17890189Poeppel D, Idsardi WJ, van Wassenhove V (March 2008). "Speech perception at the interface of neurobiology and linguistics". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 363 (1493): 1071–86. doi:10.1098/rstb.2007.2160. PMC 2606797. PMID 17890189.
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[13]
Citation Link//www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20368422Newman AJ, Supalla T, Hauser P, Newport EL, Bavelier D (2010). "Dissociating neural subsystems for grammar by contrasting word order and inflection". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 107 (16): 7539–44. Bibcode:2010PNAS..107.7539N. doi:10.1073/pnas.1003174107. PMC 2867749. PMID 20368422.
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[14]
Citation Linkopenlibrary.orgWernicke K. (1995). "The aphasia symptom-complex: A psychological study on an anatomical basis (1875)". In Paul Eling (ed.). Reader in the History of Aphasia: From sasi(Franz Gall to). 4. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Pub Co. pp. 69–89. ISBN 978-90-272-1893-3.
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[15]
Citation Linkarchive.orgDronkers NF.; Redfern B B.; Knight R T. (2000). "The neural architecture of language disorders". In Bizzi, Emilio; Gazzaniga, Michael S. (eds.). The New cognitive neurosciences (2nd ed.). Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. pp. 949–58. ISBN 978-0-262-07195-6.
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[16]
Citation Link//www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12615649Saygin AP, Dick F, Wilson SM, Dronkers NF, Bates E (2003). "Neural resources for processing language and environmental sounds: evidence from aphasia". Brain. 126 (Pt 4): 928–45. doi:10.1093/brain/awg082. PMID 12615649.
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[17]
Citation Linkmeshb.nlm.nih.govD065813
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[18]
Citation Linkbraininfo.rprc.washington.edu1233
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[19]
Citation Linkwww.neurolex.orgnlx_144087
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM
[20]
Citation Linkbioportal.bioontology.org242178
Sep 26, 2019, 2:14 PM